Category Archives: Critical Thinking

Calls for Justice and Truth

Perhaps due to social media or 24-hour news, there seem to be more and more people loudly protesting for justice, truth, or inquiries to achieve those. Justice and truth are needed in a democratic, decent society. Yet not everyone who calls for those necessarily supports those, even when they believe they do.

That may sound harsh but that doesn’t make it untrue. These people are not liars. They are sometimes mistaken and misled by a part of our thinking increasingly revealed through science. If those words anger you, you may be proving my points below. If you don’t read on from here, you just did.

Protests and public discourse
Public outcry is an inherent right in free societies. Protests led to the creation nations such as the United States, and governing systems in countries across the globe. Although Afro-centric groups have been front and centre in media the past two years, other groups step out and protest too.

Protesters loudly called for justice after former CBC host Gian Gomeshi was acquitted of all counts in his sexual assault case. And, long before the so called Ferguson effect, lots of other groups loudly called for justice and truth, or inquiries to get those. Following the G20 summit in Toronto, for instance, there were many calls for public inquiries into police actions and government direction of those actions. Therefore, calls for justice and the like not a Black thing, they are a human thing. I support these principles.

In the movie An American President, screenwriter Aaron Sorkin had the title character speak of these principles in a press conference,

You want free speech? Let’s see you acknowledge a man whose words make your blood boil whose standing centre stage and advocating at the top of his lungs that which you would oppose for a lifetime at the top of yours…then you can sing about the land of the free.

Freedom and democracy are messy things. Winston Churchill wrote (in Churchill by Himself), “Indeed it has been said that democracy is the worst form of Government except for all those other forms that have been tried from time to time…” What makes that form of government better, in my view, are people seeking the truth as best and accurately as they can.

Where the problems start
Not everyone is necessarily correct in their calls for justice. Volume,  numbers, and repetition do not themselves determine the truth of any argument. Undefined terms commonly fuel misunderstanding and disagreement. Justice is often undefined.

The Oxford English Dictionary (OED) defines “justice” in part as “(1) Just behaviour or treatment”, and “(1.1) The quality of being fair and reasonable”. For this discussion I define “reasonable” as that which can be supported by objective facts and determined by impartial, unbiased assessment of those facts. I hope you agree.

I previously wrote in a few posts about the importance of fixed principles in reasoning. Justice and reasonableness are fixed principles. They do not favour any belief system, political cause, or group. Documented reasoning emerged in Ancient Greece long before this part of the globe was even known (outside of the Indigenous peoples living here, that is). Our justice system predates today’s issues by centuries. Obviously, there are some miscarriages of justice as revealed by groups like the Association for the Defence of the Wrongfully Convicted (ADWYC). Nevertheless, the system is designed to be as transparent and fair as possible. ADWYC has achieved dozens of reversals through that justice system. No judge would say the system is perfect. The existence of appellant courts speaks to this, as do laws evolving through legislation.

Homosexuality was unlawful in many countries until mere decades ago yet today that seems draconian to the vast majority of people in the Western world regardless of their sexual preferences. Racial segregation on buses was the law in places like Montgomery, Alabama, until the U.S. Supreme Court struck it down in Bowder v. Gayles in 1956. That same court outlawed the ability of the Skokie (Illinois) town council to ban Nazis from marching in their town in 1977, despite many Holocaust survivors living there. In the latter case, the American Civil Liberty Union (ACLU) defended the Nazis because the principles of free speech and assembly are enshrined in their Constitution. More to the point, the ACLU defended their Constitution despite who it benefitted in that case.

So it goes with justice and truth. Sometimes, it makes one group happy and another miserable. Sometimes it helps people we revile but if justice and truth are important to us, so be  it. Sometimes the truth sucks, but that doesn’t make it untrue. In precisely the same way, sometimes the results of justice leave us miserable but that doesn’t make something unjust.

In a previous post, I referenced simultaneous sets of findings by the U.S. Department of Justice concerning Ferguson, Missouri. One set of findings was that systemic racial discrimination existed within the policing and justice systems in Ferguson. The other set cleared then officer Darren Wilson in the shooting death of Michael Brown, stating that the shooting was justifiable. Attorney General Eric Holder cited significant evidence supporting both sets of findings, including disgusting emails showing racial discrimination, and significant forensic evidence corroborating Wilson’s version of events.

Many people rejected findings about systemic racism but cheered for the officer being cleared. Others cheered the first findings while rejecting the second. Put another way, many people employed in Ferguson to administer the law chose to ignore findings by the highest organization in their country for administering the law, and many people who called for justice chose to ignore the findings of their federal department of justice. That’s the problem when people adopt a set of views and thereafter reject anything that conflicts with those views no matter the facts or the people finding them. This is confirmation bias, which is demonstrated by both sides in Ferguson, and elsewhere.

Specific examples
I have no direct knowledge in two examples I provide below but each shows, as does the one I know a little bit about, an interesting aspect of human cognition examined more closely later on. I believe each group earnestly believes what they do and that they may be right, or not.

Protesters attacked the acquittal of Gian Gomeshi. Violence against women, particularly sexual assault, deserves serious attention, more so when the allegations involve three women, strangling, and punching. The case was covered in minute detail and was already analyzed in a documentary that included interviews of two alleged victims before the verdict and the other one within hours of that verdict.

Virtually every angle of this story, including the alleged victims’ interviews, suggests they withheld information relevant to the case. Defense counsel was surgical and thorough in revealing that, which is their job. Accordingly, the judge found none of the victims credible and the case could not stand without a credible victim. Victims are human and we all make mistakes. Those of us not victimized by sexual assault should allow a wide berth for those who are, and for their foibles, which we all have. The sad fact is that these women may all have been victimized by Gomeshi. He wasn’t proven innocent, but the onus is always on the prosecution to meet the burden of proof. Were it not so, any one of us could be stuck trying to prove our innocence against a large government agency. That happened for centuries elsewhere in many places, to devastating effect. Despite the safeguards in our justice system, that sometimes happens here to devastating effects too. But sometimes the system does work, and it appears to have in the Gomeshi matter despite an outcome that upsets many.

I truly hope future victims will not be scared away from reporting crimes due to what happened in this case. The outcome is upsetting to many advocates of women’s safety, and to me, but it is the only reasonable conclusion a judge could come to in the absence of credible testimony.

Loku Protesters: Black Lives Matter – Toronto Chapter protested out front of police headquarters for many days. Their recent focus has been on the 2015 shooting death of Andrew Loku by Toronto officers, and the Ontario’s Special Investigations Unit (SIU)’s determination that the shooting was justified. The protesters would tell you that Loku had a hammer (not a gun or knife) and may have been mentally in crisis when he was shot. All that appears true. The SIU believes the shooting was justified anyway and explained that in their press release.

Ontario has had police-involved incidents involving serious injury and death independently investigated since 1990. The SIU has charged several officers. Some previous SIU directors have harshly criticized police agencies on a number of fronts. A few officers have been convicted (Regina v. Forcillo and R v. Sandhu are just two cases). They have also been criticized by the court for laying charges where none were justified (R. v. Cavannagh). Accordingly, it’s an uphill argument to suggest the SIU is in league with the police. Still the protesters have rights and are availing themselves of those. I wonder why they haven’t also protested the SIU office but maybe they have and no press covered that. The absence of an event in the news doesn’t mean it didn’t happen.

Would any objective facts change the protesters’ minds? We’ll see. Have Black communities endured all manner of crap for centuries, enough to make some of their members suspicious of many patterns? Without question.

G20 Protesters (2010): This was, generously speaking, a hot mess. It was a mess during the summit due to protests, many of which were prevocational and some which were violent. After the first mess, groups took to demanding all manner of inquiries, firings, and changes. Some criminal charges and other actions followed but nowhere near as many as many protesters called for.

My involvement in this issue afforded me a closer look at the problem with some calls for justice. A month after the protests, a friend of mine emailed me a Flickr photo and accompanying narrative posted by the photographer of that pic. It railed against officers harassing other photographers and how a camera is not a weapon. I agree. Nonetheless, I replied largely because I was in the photograph.

I was briefly speaking with an officer I know personally and photographed by the Flickr contributor with that officer That’s why my friend emailed with the link. So I pointed out that that contributor was mistaken and presumptuous. One of the other officers was a photography enthusiast who asked a photographer about an $8,000+ lens he loved. I even offered to introduce the gentleman to the officer who spoke with the photographer. To his credit, he backed off and even apologized. However, he repeatedly called for a public inquiry into police actions.

I didn’t argue against that and didn’t think it was my place to. However, I asked a few times if he would accept a different view of what occurred if irrefutable facts proved those in such an inquiry. He never directly answered. He merely repeated his claims, which I took as an indirect NO. Apparently, then, this man accepted one version of events, wanted that view aired more widely, and would not accept a contrary view no matter what the facts were, no matter what the truth was.

To be fair to this man and the two protesting groups I mentioned above, this approach to the truth and justice is far more common than some may believe. Furthermore, it is on both ends of most issues.

The science on confirmation bias
In recent years, the term bias has replaced prejudiced and racist although with marginally less sting. One colleague said months ago that to be branded as racially biased is only slightly less devastating than being branded a pedophile. Because the most common form of bias discussed has been of the racial kind, racial bias is accepted as being almost everywhere. I agree. Biases of all kinds are almost everywhere, negative ones and positive ones. One of the most common forms is confirmation bias, of which I’ve written more than once in previous posts.

In Thinking Fast and Slow, Daniel Kahneman outlined many heuristics, the hallmark of fast thinking. Often, they are also the roots of bias. Our brains are hard-wired to identify patterns with which we can almost instantly react to future situations based on subconscious recognition of those patterns. In prehistoric times and many others, these helped our ancestors survive. They still help us, avoiding vehicle collisions and some other harm, for example. The down side of this fast thinking is that these patterns aren’t always correct, and there aren’t always patterns where our fast thinking seems to believe there are.

Heuristics are based on experiences, stories and well as interpretation and remembering of those. Interpretations and memory can be spot on or way off the mark but fast thinking leaves little room for doubt. With a bias in place, further experiences and stories are often viewed through that biased lens. Moreover, many of us notice more, seek more, and remember more information that confirms our biases (confirmation bias), and reject, ignore, or expend energy disproving contrary information (disconfirmation bias).

Here’s a quick story I ask that you picture in your head. Ready?

At 9 PM on September 21, 2009, a White, uniformed police officer quickly jogged up to a Black man in his early twenties, who was dressed in hip-hop attire and standing on a sidewalk. The officer looked concerned and a bit excited, perhaps even fearful. The man standing on the sidewalk was panting slightly with his hands on his hips standing upright, and he appeared slightly agitated or angered. That’s it, the end of my story.

“Huh,” you might wonder, “where does the story go from here?” For a lot of readers, your fast thinking brain already started finishing the story, filling in the blanks. Bias often creates expectations of what will most likely happen next. Did you?

I’ve used this technique in classroom for demonstration. Most people who exhibited a pro-police or pro-White bias anticipated that the officer would arrest the man for some crime he just committed and fled from. Most people with the opposite biases anticipated that the officer would harass an innocent man. Bias and the heuristics don’t usually seek more information unless of the confirming kind. Bias and heuristics tend to say, as Kahneman put words to these subconscious processes, “What you see is all there is.”

Fortunately, some people said there was nowhere near enough information to anticipate what happened next. Most of those people likely had biases, being human beings and all. But they also engaged their slow thinking. This part of thinking often says, in my words, “Hold on. There could be more here. Who knows what that is or what it will mean?”

There are many ways my story could play out. Blatant police harassment of a visible minority. A justified arrest of a bad guy, his skin colour being irrelevant. Officer approaching a victim who just fled an attack. Officer coming to help a colleague in street clothes or working undercover, who was just attacked or who just chased a suspect. Officer approaching a witness who chased a suspect and lost him. The list of possibilities goes on but bias narrows conceived possibilities to a few, and sometimes to only one.

Stopping long-ingrained biases from narrowing our consideration is not easy for people, especially if we do not routinely use slow, deliberate thinking. It takes longer and it takes more mental energy. It doesn’t satisfy an innately human characteristic, wanting to be certain all the time. It doesn’t satisfy the emotional responses which fast thinking often kick start. However, it tends to point us to more accurate, truthful conclusions.

Writing about confirmation bias on the website You’re Not So Smart, David McRaney wrote, “Your opinions are the result of years of paying attention to information which confirmed what you believed while ignoring information which challenged your preconceived notions.”

McRaney cited many studies, only a few mentioned here. Valdis Krebs analyzed purchasing patterns of people liking or reviling (then) candidate Obama in 2008. The data suggested that buyers did not seek information, merely confirmation. A University of a Minnesota study in 2008 showed that subjects focused on information supporting their assertion while ignoring or forgetting contrary information. An Ohio State University study, in 2009, showed subjects spending 36 per cent more time reading essays that “aligned with their opinion”. Yet the next study McRaney mentioned is truly eye-popping: “Another study at Ohio State in 2009 showed subjects clips of the parody show ‘The Colbert Report,’ and people who considered themselves politically conservative consistently reported ‘Colbert only pretends to be joking and genuinely meant what he said.’”

Um, wow! If you’re not familiar with Stephen Colbert’s former show, The Colbert Report, take a look at any YouTube clip from his show and see if you think he believes what he says in it.

You might think someone would have to be an idiot to think Colbert was doing anything other than shredding conservatives in that clip, whichever one you may have just watched. But science shows us, time and again, that when we allow ourselves to be ruled by our subconscious then our beliefs, our words and actions can look fairly odd to people not in lock step with that particular bias. And, those people will seem strange or misinformed or stupid or lying to us if or when confirmation bias grips us.

Education seems not to be an automatic guard against that, unless that education included explicit training in critical thought (largely the basis of slow thinking), and those graduates routinely practice that thinking in their daily lives.

Just like every other aspect of humanity, confirmation bias is not a Black thing, a poor thing, or a left-wing thing. Read up on McCarthyism if you want to see the confirmation bias on the right, or view a Trump rally. Anti-police advocates are correct that police officers are inherently biased in any number of ways. Police officers are homo-sapiens, all of whom are predisposed toward heuristics, and bias. Of course, their critics are also homo-sapiens so they are not immune from all manner of biases either. As Chris Mooney wrote, for MotherJones.com (denial of science), “We apply fight-or-flight reflexes not only to predators, but to data itself.” That’s the bad news.

The good news about the bad news
As Kahneman pointed out, slow thinking exists but is not our default setting. He too is subject to heuristics but he tries to catch himself and think things out a little more. This is what more-recent researchers refer to as dual processing. In dynamic or rapidly evolving situations, slow thinking will not always be a viable option. This requires that we practice what Brent Snook (Memorial University) calls bounded rationality. As Snook and some judges have noted, it is not reasonable to expect people in rapidly dynamic situations to calmly, rationally drill down on our subconscious thoughts or instincts. As Aaron Barth, assistant professor in philosophy at The University of Western Ontario and principal owner of Dialectic Strategies recently commented, it is not reasonable to judge System 1 (fast thinking) decisions exclusively with System 2 (slow thinking) when circumstances the judged person was in precluded them using  their System 2.

When circumstances are not as scary and immediate, though, it serves us well to do two things:

  1. Learn in advance how to think reasonably in the truest sense of the word.
  2. Practice that kind of thinking regularly so it has a fighting chance when our emotionally-driven, narrow-minded, selectively-listening, fast thinking automatically kicks in. It is possible and it is sometimes demonstrated.

Many people will recall the 2013 trial of George Zimmerman in the shooting death of Black teenager Trayvon Martin, who was unarmed. Lead defense counsel Mark O’Mara became of the face of the successful defense of Zimmerman, who few would describe as a man who did everything right, O’Mara included. Following the trial, Zimmerman was vilified in the press and by advocacy groups, and his subsequent antics haven’t helped his cause. O’Mara was somewhat vilified too.

Since then, O’Mara has been an occasional legal contributor on CNN. Whereas some legal commentators have shown a decided bias for or against police, O’Mara has been remarkably unpredictable, at least to those who expect every incident involving Black people interacting with people in authority to have happened the way they expect it to have.

In July of 2015 Samuel DuBose, an unarmed Black motorist, was shot dead by a University of Cincinnati officer who has since been charged in the case. When the DuBose family wanted help on the legal front, they reached out to O’Mara, who is now assisting the family.

Intuitively, O’Mara might seem an odd choice. How could a man who defended the killer of a Black teen do an about-face and represent the family of a killed Black man? Through the lens of fast thinking, through bias, O’Mara might be viewed as previously defending the killing of Black people. In actual fact, he defended one man in one case and he is now representing the family of another man in another case, one where a man was shot in circumstances that appear significantly different than the Zimmerman matter. Similarly, O’Mara ventures various opinions on CNN based on the known-facts of each matter under discussion, just like several other respected counsel do.

O’Mara might be viewed as unpredictable in that he pleases some advocates on one matter and may alienate those same advocates on other matters. Yet I suspect his decisions are not guided by any group or agenda. Rather, his decisions appear to be made on a case-by-case analysis of the known facts. By the same principle, the DuBose family may have shown considerably open minds by seeking out O’Mara despite his involvement in the Zimmerman trial. Their choice suggests that some every-day people can and do make decisions on a case-by-case basis too.

Summary
O’Mara and the DuBose family may have provided an excellent example of the best way to mitigate bias. View each issue, each matter, each argument, individually. Weigh out the objective facts as best as they are known to you at the time, the ones which support your initial belief and the ones opposing it. If you want to, seek out more information on either side of the ledger. Then form an opinion. All the while, remember that your opinion could be wrong despite your best efforts to get it right.

Trying our best to get it right is within our grasps. It isn’t easy. Like driving, it takes learning, practice, experience, as well as learning about others’ mistakes and the bad outcomes stemming from those. Just as there are bad drivers who think they are good ones, there are poor thinkers who believe they are sound ones. As with driving, thinking skills can be improved and it is in our inherent best interests to do that. Wrote Clyde Miller in his essay, How to Detect Propaganda,

Why are we fooled by these devices? Because they appeal to our emotions rather than to our reason. They make us believe and do something we would not believe or do if we thought about it calmly, dispassionately. In examining these devices, note that they work most effectively at those times when we are too lazy to think for ourselves, also, they tie into emotions which sway us to be “for” or “against” nations, races, religions, ideals, economic and political policies and practices, and so on through automobiles, cigarettes, radios, toothpastes, presidents, and wars. With our emotions stirred, it may be fun to be fooled by these devices, but it is more fun and infinitely more to our own interests to know how they work.

There seems to be little point in trying to go head-to-head with someone’s beliefs when they are heavily invested in those being true, at almost any cost. As Chris Mooney wrote, “Head-on attempts to persuade can sometimes trigger a backfire effect, where people not only fail to change their minds when confronted with the facts – they may hold their wrong views more tenaciously than ever.”

In my view, the demonstrators against Gomeshi’s acquittal, and the Black Lives Matter Toronto demonstrators, and the previously angry photographer I had a dialogue with could each be right about their beliefs, perhaps a little and perhaps more. Or they could be wrong  but they don’t appear inclined to consider that. Very few people are right or wrong about everything. Confirmation bias gets in the way of us realizing that. Confirmation bias knows no sides or ideologies or faiths. It knows only that it wants to be right no matter what, as if the belief holder’s existence hinges on that belief being right. It rarely does unless we make it so.

I wouldn’t try to dissuade these people from their deeply held beliefs; it would be a fool’s errand. I’m not sure if any of those people would be willing to admit the possibility of them being mistaken. I know the photographer never showed me that ability. What I hope to do with this post and others works is to encourage readers to start questioning their own beliefs, or to at least work up the willingness to do that, and no matter whether they agree with me on any given issue or not.

Ideas are supposed to be about that; ideas, not people. Ideas can change if we let them. By extension, people can change too, provided their willing to try new ideas. So, the next time you ask a question, ask yourself if you are seeking information to reasonably consider or merely confirmation of your already arrived-at conclusion.

Please, let me know where I’m wrong here if you believe I am.  Sometimes, I am mistaken. How about you?

Conspiracy Theories

Conspiracy theories are nothing new. Today’s list includes 9/11, “chemtrails” (versus contrails), genetically modified foods (GMOs), fluoride in drinking water, and the new world order. Older ones like the the worldwide Jewish conspiracy and one of The Beatles being replaced by a doppleganger persist but they currently take a back seat. The tragic Ebola outbreak in West Africa was raging in 2015. At least one alternative media source claimed it was a conspiracy between the American government and big pharma.

 

Many of these theories seem to be rubbish to a lot of people, although precise numbers and ratios are unreliable. A few of my friends and relatives accept some of those theories. Two of my cousins, for instance, believe the moon landings were a hoax and I respect them greatly. I tend not to believe in that conspiracy but I have no concrete proof to the contrary. Like everyone outside the inner workings of NASA, I must rely on news and historical reports to tell me what happened. Could we have been lied to by some sources? Sure.

 

When I refer to someone in this piece as a conspiracy theorist, I mean someone who believes in, or theorizes on, several conspiracies. I use the term as a description, not an insult.

 

I have a friend I will call Bill, which isn’t his name. He believes in all the theories I mentioned in my first paragraph, excluding the Jewish conspiracy stuff. I have trouble buying any of those, despite how passionately and confidently he speaks of them. Still, there are several noteworthy aspects to Bill that I must and do consider.

 

I cannot think of one time he has lied to anyone, even a little, even if it would help him,  even when it was awkward. He’s repeatedly called me an idiot or a sheep, to my face, for not being in lock-step with him and his beliefs. Yet he also provides me valuable information and helps me in tangible ways. Thus, I conclude he is trying to help me even when he attacks me.

 

He is a passionate salesman for all sorts of beliefs but stands to gain nothing personally from me buying into those. He doesn’t sell any products, canvas for any donations, push any memberships, or try to be the popular guy. That makes his positions less suspect than one guy at a home show, who once told me of the evils and toxicity of conventional candles only to try selling me bees wax candles from his booth moments later.

 

I believe in consistency of principle and practice. In a previous post, in my recent book, and elsewhere, I warned against the dangers of absolutes. Stating an absolute is most often a fallacy too. Few beliefs are absolutely right or wrong, good or bad, true or hogwash. So, Bill frustrates me when he implies that all his beliefs are 100% correct but I would be a hypocrite if I believed that everything he believes is 100% false. Accordingly, I believe that some of the sources Bill believes may be correct, perhaps somewhat and perhaps a lot. I simply have no idea or opinion on which ones.

 

The term conspiracy describes an action planned and/or executed among two or more people. A robber and a getaway driver form a conspiracy. The Holocaust was a massive conspiracy. The Normandy invasion was a conspiracy as were almost all military actions since the dawn of time. So too are efforts of family and friends to get addicts into treatment as portrayed on the television show Intervention. Not all conspiracies are unlawful or evil or selfish but some certainly are. Such is human nature that many people are fairly good but some are very bad, particularly if left unchecked. History has shown far too many examples of unchecked badness, sometimes individually and sometimes by a team effort. Nonetheless, not every bad outcome or even most needs a conspiracy to make it happen.

 

In Thinking, Fast and Slow, which I referenced in an earlier post, Daniel Kahneman described intuitive thought, how rapidly it jumps to conclusions, and how steadfastly confident most of us feel about any conclusions from such intuitions even when they are wrong. He also explained how mistaken intuition can be, and how it can be mitigated by deliberative, critical thought. He revealed research showing how common intuitive thought is, and how firmly many mistaken beliefs are held when produced by intuition. Putting words to this often-subconscious process, Kahneman described it as, “What you see is all there is”.

 

The far less common thought process, System 2,requires training, formally or informally, as well as a questioning one’s own intuitive beliefs. That is no easy task. We all have intuitions but far fewer mitigate those with reason. The central element of fast thinking, of intuition, is that it produces heuristics. Our brains produce these to save time and energy, often subconsciously. And, while not all heuristics are bad and some of can save us in times of need, they can also cause us many problems.

 

When it comes to conspiracy theories, a common heuristic appears to play a part: guessing at patterns. With this heuristic, we seek to find patterns that may not exist, especially in preference to chance or randomness. Georgia State University has a handy list of common heuristics for easy reference.

 

On April 14, 1912 over 1,500 passengers of the RMS Titanic died within hours of it striking an ice berg in the North Atlantic. Multiple examinations revealed that over a dozen factors contributed to that disaster. Many of them were human errors committed by many strangers over the course of more than a decade, from the people who passed the life boat regulations which were based on ship tonnage instead of the number of passengers to a sloppy evacuation and so on. Nevertheless, it was the combination of those elements that resulted in the disaster.

 

Disaster Theory suggests that in the case of many bad events, even one less contributing factor can sometimes avert tragedy. It can be quite discomforting to think that a series of disconnected mistakes randomly strung together can result in such tragedy, that stupidity randomly coupled with luck can result in over a thousand deaths. So some people seek out or believe theories that seem to provide some unifying explanation. Some people seem to feel less comfortable with randomness and chance than with the belief of ubiquitous treachery. And yes, there are conspiracy theories for the Titanic.

 

Fast forward to September 11, 2001: Investigations by the U.S. government and others, such as the magazine Popular Mechanics, provided explanations that combine evil acts with sloppy security and investigative work, a design flaw in the architecture of the World Trade Center and other factors. Yet some people, including a conglomeration of 2,200 plus engineers and architects, theorize that it was a conspiracy involving controlled demolition and other elements. The thinking among those theorists and other self-called truthers is that there is no way things could happen the way their government says it did, there is no way so many disparate things and random chance could have gone wrong over many years. The trouble is, sometimes they do. Were horrible events of 9/11 a conspiracy? The U.S. government says it was, carried out by Al Qaeda and aided by lots of errors. I can’t prove either the truthers’ conspiracy theories or government accounts. I do not, however, buy the impossibility of  randomness. That would be a fallacy as well as a heuristic.

 

My trouble with conspiracy theorists, those who believe in many of them and mockingly dismiss any contrary information (known as confirmation heuristic), is precisely that, how they dismiss or attack any question of their beliefs. When you add emotion (the appeal to emotion fallacy) and personalized attacks (the ad hominem fallacy), I get even more suspicious. Nevertheless, I have to stop myself from rejecting everything from such people out of hand, because that would itself be biased. Besides, some of these conspiracies could be real. Evil has been done by many people and groups and governments, and thereafter hidden, sometimes for decades, centuries, or maybe forever.

 

As the oft-quoted Edmund Burke said, “All that is required for the triumph of evil is that good men do nothing”. Bill’s sometimes angry words are simply his attempt to do something, to wake some people up, including me. It’s hard not to admire his moral conviction, and his passion. Historically, many people who were derided as liars, imbeciles, or lunatics turned out to be correct. Galileo Galilei (1564 – 1642), to name just one, was forced by his country and church to recant his theories. He lived the rest of his life under house arrest. Nevertheless, he wrote books of his theories while in captivity, those theories were later proven true, and they formed the foundation of modern astronomy and physics.

 

I feel pretty sure that the moon landings happened, and my suspicious cousins have no direct knowledge to prove otherwise. The problem is that I have no direct knowledge to prove they did happen. In the absence of our beliefs being proven beyond all reasonable doubt, we must each meander through the most reliable sources we can, reasonably question what we know, and navigate our lives as best we can. The same holds true for Bill, for millions of others, and for me.

 

Bill and I agree that we should question what goes on in the world. We just need to be open minded enough to consider information from various sources, and weigh the relative credibility and reliability of those. Not all books or web sites are equally credible. Hitler wrote a book and ISIS runs web sites. By contrast, Bill is a good guy with good intentions so I still listen to him. I hope he occasionally listens to me too. I may be an idiot, but in a way, at least I’m his idiot.

 

What You Know

Let’s look at what you know, and what you think you know.

 

Even if I don’t know you personally, which is highly likely, I still have a fairly good idea of what you know about a specific topic I introduce in a moment. That will probably reveal a lot about what you know more broadly.

 

• For the last 22 years, I have participated in tug of war.

 

Without reading any further than the end of this sentence, take a moment or two and think about what that looks like, the tug of war part, not me.

 

Depending on many variables, ranging from your level of general knowledge to where you grew up and live to what types of employers you have had, you likely have a pre-formed picture of what tug of war is. I excluded putting an image related to tug of war atop this blog post, to avoid influencing whatever that pre-formed picture is.

 

Considering that I’ve participated in it so long, you can probably appreciate that I’ve spoken of it with many hundreds of people. From those conversations, I believe i have an accurate picture of what most people in many countries know to be “tug of war”, a term well-known in many parts of the globe. From those experiences I believe that virtually everyone who speaks English “knows” what tug of war is, and that more than 95% of them are incorrect. Thus, it is highly probable you are incorrect too. Let me tell you some more about this very narrow topic:

 

• I’ve competed internationally against teams from as far away as the Netherlands, South Korea, Latvia, Malaysia, and South Africa, to name a few.
• One of my team mates has a Ph.D., used to work in medical research, and is currently a professor in exercise sciences.
• The last (outdoor) World Championships of Tug of War were in the United States. The ones before that were in Switzerland and the Netherlands respectively. The next ones are in Sweden. They run every two years.
• I sometimes drop roughly 25 pounds, and other team mates drop weight too, in order for us to meet the weight requirements to compete in two different divisions, each which has a maximum weight under which a team of eight competitors must weigh to qualify. This is very common in our sport.
• In an average tournament, it is common to pull against several opposing teams for a combined number of more than 20 pulls within a few hours.
• Pulls, or “ends” as we call them, go until one team loses. Thus, an end can take 10 seconds or a few minutes, although the latter feels like it is much longer.
• Tug of war has been an international sport for decades, and there are over 50 national associations within the international governing body, the Tug of War International Federation (TWIF). I am part of the Canadian Tug of War Association (CATOWA); yes, that’s a thing.

 

Now consider for another moment what you now know tug of war to be. Do those facts fit what your first image of the activity was? In all likelihood it has changed and yet it may still be incorrect, to some extent at least. A better word to use than “know”, for what you picture on this topic and many others, might be “believe”.

 

My knowledge and experience in this narrow topic allowed me to learn a lot about it. Tug of war is an ancient sport but one which faded from wide public knowledge and interest over the last century and a half, during which many high profile and well-known sports emerged. The tug of war tradition has two sets of historical roots; one is in agricultural communities, the other in military or paramilitary organizations.

 

If you come from a rural area where there is or once was a regularly competitive tug of war club, or you were once a soldier or police officer and your organization had a team, you may have a more accurate picture of the sport than most people. Even then, most tug of war teams come from a fairly small regional section of their countries. For example, most U.S. teams are from Minnesota, Wisconsin, and Iowa, and most Canadian teams from southwestern Ontario or rural Nova Scotia, right now at least.

 

The vast majority of people, and probably you, pictured a picnic activity that is over in about 30 seconds, maybe with a mud pit in the middle or with children only. Is that what you initially pictured? If so, that’s understandable for some of the reasons I already provided. What you imagined or pictured or thought of constituted what you believed and, at this moment, that may be changing if only somewhat.

 

Even some people who have competed in a well-organized tug of war tournament, or several, may not have a full grasp of the topic. Tug of war tournaments have taken place annually for well over two centuries at a fair in Windsor, Nova Scotia. Police officers from the Marion County Sheriff’s Office in Oakala, Florida used to compete in tug of war in the World Police and Fire Games. Nevertheless, neither group have competed against each other or even heard of each other or competed in TWIF sanctioned events. So, their knowledge of the topic of tug of war may go well beyond children’s games and picnics yet still fall short of the full scoop. In fairness and contrast, when teams of children or picnic goers pull against another team on a rope, even for just a minute or less, that too is tug of war in the same way that a few kids playing shinny on a frozen pond are still playing hockey.

 

Some people’s initial picture of tug of war is a lot less complimentary than others. Some people initially know tug of war to be done in only a few areas, quickly, once in a blue moon, and by oversized dummies. That’s why I included the second bullet point above, about my team mate being a professor, to contradict a few stereotypes and challenge yours if you had those.

 

If you never again think about tug of war, fair enough. But here’s the point of my providing you a crash course on a little known sport: is tug of war the only topic about which you know a lot less than you think? Is it the only topic about which your beliefs are based on incomplete or absent information?

 

Tug of war enthusiasts often see the reactions from people when they learn, usually in casual conversation, that we participate in tug of war. Sometimes the reaction is confusion or questions, which are great either way. Sometimes it is laughter, presumably at the thought of grown adults playing at a kid’s game. When people laugh at us being in tug of war, we often laugh too, but at them not with them.

 

The next time you go to make a decision, whether a purchase or voting or expressing a poorly-informed opinion, you may want to consider that. LOL?

Blog 12 pic

 

Is Ignorance Bliss?

I write and teach frequently about thought processes, and I often use hypothetical situations to illustrate this point or that. Today I have a real-life scenario that highlights the question posed by this blog entry’s title.

 

A few months ago, a young lady I’ll refer to as Susan (not her real name) texted me with a few questions in an effort to help her friend, who I neither know nor know the name of. I’ll call her Madge and pray that isn’t her real name.

 

Continue reading Is Ignorance Bliss?

One Horrible Incident and the Big Pictures Stemming from It

As most people in North America know, on August 9, 2014 an unarmed male, Michael Brown, was shot to death by police officer Darren Wilson in Ferguson, Missouri. These facts are not in dispute. Almost everything else is. Although investigations are now concluded, many beliefs have remained so I refer to those in the present tense. Without question, this was a horrible incident from which have sprung several big pictures, some spoken of and some not.

 

On their face, these facts look very bad on the officer involved. He has since voluntarily left his job and currently lives in hiding, in large part due to death threats. Legions of people rushed to judge him guilty, and those people may be absolutely correct, or not. Sadly, I realize how upsetting those last two words may be to some readers and plenty of others.

 

Without sufficient information, how could I truly know at that time what happened or form reasonable opinions about that? Two large investigations since concluded with no charges being laid against Wilson. The first saw the local grand jury decline to indict the officer, but many if not most people rejected that process as racist or rigged by the district attorney’s presentation to the jurors. Many legal commentators criticized that process. Months later, the U.S. Department of Justice (DOJ) released their own findings on the case as well as their review of the Ferguson Police Department. They too found there are insufficient grounds to charge Wilson in the death of Michael Brown, stating that Wilson was legally justified in believing he had to use lethal force to avoid death or grievance harm from Michael Brown.

 

It is more difficult to dismiss the DOJ findings as racist or rigged because they simultaneously released a scathing report of racial bias, including blatantly racist emails, within the Ferguson PD and local justice system. Those findings of systemic racism led some people, including some high-profile legal contributors on cable news shows, to say that systemic racism must mean Wilson was guilty of something.

 

The simple and largely-ignored fact is that thousands if not millions of people reached their decisions about Wilson’s actions, most against him and a small minority for him, within hours or a few days of the incident itself. The first big picture here, and a troubling one, is that many people in both groups formed their beliefs through bias.

 

In a previous post on this blog, I addressed the general concept of bias. I drill a bit deeper into that here, if only so we can collectively lose the belief that bias is only something held by those with whom we disagree.

 

In his ground-breaking book Thinking, Fast and Slow, Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman examined two vastly different thinking systems he referred to, for simplicity, as Systems 1 and 2. System 2 is the slow kind where reasoning and internal considerations affect our decisions. Using this system requires deliberation, cognitive self-discipline, a reigning in of emotion, and considerable learning (formally or informally), none of which are as common as we may like to believe. System 1 is the fast kind informed by our previous knowledge and experiences, our interpretations of those, and often affected by what we have heard about other people’s experiences. Much of this influence is subconscious. This is the common type of thought everyone engages in, Kahneman included, and which some people almost exclusively engage in.

 

As Kahneman explained, System 1 can make the right choice in many circumstances, often without conscious thought. Our instincts and intuitions and automatic responses are from System 1. They often help us and sometimes save our lives. For example, many drivers automatically react to immediate road hazards by steering around them, often without recognizing any thought process whatsoever despite those thoughts happening in an instant. This type of thought was also the basis of Malcolm Gladwell’s Blink: The Power of Thinking without Thinking.

 

The trouble is that decisions produced by System 1 are ill informed and not always correct, even when they feel very correct. As both Kahneman and Gladwell pointed out in different ways, fast thinking is the source of our biases. Since everyone engages in fast thinking, everyone has biases, some which are very helpful in many circumstances but certainly not all. That principle applies equally to police officers and to their critics, and to everyone else with a brain.

 

Looking back at the Ferguson shooting, you and I first heard about it within hours or a few days. The initial information was very limited about an incident perhaps one minute long with a young person dying as a result. To thousands or millions, deadly actions against an unarmed person in such a short time must have resulted from racial or professional or class biases. Accordingly, the actions of Officer Wilson were and still are viewed by many people as being biased and, therefore, wrong. Issues of contention in this matter even included whether the 18-year-old victim should be referred to as a ‘child’, as many vehemently professed, or as a ‘man’, which others equally professed. That’s why I earlier referred to Michael Brown as “an unarmed male”, to be as neutral and precise as possible.

 

If you formed a belief against or for the officer, how long did it take you to reach it? If it happened in minutes or even a few days, it was likely anchored in bias simply because there was insufficient information to accurately assess the truth of that matter, and not because I say so but because the investigating authorities did.

 

Ambiguity is difficult for some people to live with. Many people faced with insufficient or conflicting information will pick the interpretation of an event most consistent with their previous beliefs, thereafter accepting as further proof any information that bolsters their beliefs and rejecting as nonsense any information that conflicts with those beliefs. This practice, often subconscious, is what confirmation bias refers to. This leads to the second big picture: many people viewed events in Ferguson through a lens of confirmation bias, but not all of them.

 

Avoiding bias and confirmation bias are the hallmarks of reasonable people and reasonable professionals. The second post-mortem of the victim was done at the Brown family’s request by renowned New York medical examiner Michael Baden. In a press conference following his examination, Dr. Baden referred to a wound at the top of Brown’s head: “It can be because he’s giving up, or because he’s charging forward at the officer”. Baden went on to say, “We need more information”. One of the Brown family’s lawyers, Benjamin L. Crump, soon after said, “The sheer number of bullets and the way they were scattered all over his body showed this police officer had a brazen disregard for the very people he was supposed to protect in that community”. Crump may be right but his comments contradicted the ambiguous preliminary findings of the medical examiner he or the Brown family chose, at the same podium and press conference.http://www.nytimes.com/2014/08/18/us/michael-brown-autopsy-shows-he-was-shot-at-least-6-times.html?_r=0

 

In fairness to Crump and the other lawyers assisting the Brown family, his job and his inclination were to fiercely advocate for a family who endured a terrible loss. Still, few pundits were as closely associated to the emotions and stresses of this terrible incident as Crump was yet many were no more unbiased in voicing their opinions. This leads to the third big picture, that experts are not necessarily immune from bias or confirmation bias.

 

To repeat, what author and researcher Kahneman referred to as System 2 (thinking) requires deliberation, cognitive self-discipline, and considerable learning. Such thinking is known as reasoning or critical thinking in other contexts and is basis for science and law among other fields. Hence, reasoning is the foundation of the adversarial legal system in countries like United States so every law student in the U.S. undergoes significant training in it. The news networks have law professionals trained and experienced in the business of reasoning yet some of them demonstrated considerable bias, and confirmation bias, in their legal commentaries. One could argue that these are academic exercises of little import. Public outcry, demonstrations, and outright violence in places from Ferguson to Seattle to Toronto, and regardless of skin colour and the issue, suggest otherwise.

 

As a fairly decent human being, I want the truth of what happened in Ferguson, Missouri to be out for all to see. If racist or otherwise bad practices contributed to Brown’s death, that needed to be exposed. The DOJ report certainly accused the Ferguson PD of both, and that needs to be effectively addressed. As with many massive investigations, ambiguities were revealed in the death of Michael Brown, primarily in the form of differing and often contradictory eye witness accounts. Some of those accounts were likely affected by bias. Some may have been fabrications. The physical evidence, however, clearly supported a version of events favouring Officer Wilson as per the DOJ investigation. Some accounts to the media differed from what those same people later told investigators and/or a grand jury. This leads to the fourth big picture, that some people make honest mistakes.

 

Professor Elizabeth Loftus and other researchers previously demonstrated that human memory is malleable, easily influenced by outside input such as hearing other people’s accounts of what happened. This unconscious process causes some people to process aspects of other people’s accounts as their own memories. Anecdotal experiences led police investigators, lawyers, and judges, decades ago, to the same conclusions as demonstrated by their long established preferences for witnesses to not speak to one another about their recollections, or hear each other’s recollections in any forum prior to providing those for the record.

 

Where the accounts of dozens of people were widely reported in the media long before witnesses were interviewed or examined, unintentional errors were bound to mix with what may have been intentional efforts to mislead. That scientifically demonstrated principle no doubt applied to some witnesses whose versions changed on small details between earlier and later versions, and whether their evidence favoured Wilson and not. That may also have applied to small differences between Wilson’s initial interview and his grand jury testimony much later. This isn’t favouring one version over another. It is merely applying a principle fairly on both sides of an issue. That too, will be upsetting to some people and it forms the fifth big picture: some people view as fair only those practices that result in what they want to hear or want to see happen.

 

To my point, many people hailed the accuracy and fairness of the DOJ report on systemic racial bias in Ferguson while also ignoring or rejecting the same organization’s findings on Darren Wilson’s lack of criminality in the death of Michael Wilson. In my view, people should reject or accept both findings since they came from the same investigative agency if not some of the same investigators too. The Ferguson administration has thus far refused to fully accept the DOJ findings about them, so far at least. Similarly, many have refused to accept the DOJ findings on the death of Michael Brown.

 

Then there is the issue of statistical analysis. Disproportionate representations of some groups within criminal justice systems has been revealed in many places over the last ten or fifteen years, yet the Ferguson numbers are so lop-sided that they seem to paint a truly damning picture. Putting the Ferguson numbers aside, which are hopefully anomalous in their extremes, racial disproportionalities still exist elsewhere and not just in the United States. More than a decade ago, the largest Toronto newspaper ran a series of articles on the issue, and public policies are still changing in response to those numbers. This reveals the sixth big picture, one rarely spoken of, that there can be more than one cause for trends occurring.

 

In Toronto and elsewhere, there exists an implied presumption that where the numbers of police contacts of any racial group are higher than their proportion of the population, the sole cause for such differences is racial bias by police officers. Reasonably speaking, that possibility cannot be rejected; however, reason also obligates us to look for other possible causes. What could possibly be a cause of racially disproportionate police contacts other than racially biased police? Perhaps details from individual incidents, or crime trends in certain areas, or a host of other variables.

 

It would be ludicrous to suggest that every police contact between them and disproportionately represented groups was perfectly above board and justified by the individual circumstances. There are more than enough individual examples spanning decades to suggest otherwise, in many places. The wrongful conviction of Rueben “Hurricane” Carter comes to mind for me. To me, though, it seems equally ludicrous to suggest that no contact between police and certain racial groups has been legitimate.

 

If some of those contacts were legitimate, how many were? What proportion of the disproportionality was legitimate? These are not rhetorical questions. They may not realistically be answerable either. Still, they matter because they involve the truth, and the best policy decisions are based on truth as best as that can be reasonably and reliably determined.

 

This brings us back to the original incident, the killing of Michael Brown. Is it possible that racial bias or other bad influences affected the actions of Darren Wilson? In light of recent DOJ findings about the Ferguson Police Department, it seems reasonable to consider that as a possibility. Is it probable that Wilson’s actions were racially motivated? The DOJ investigators, who had more access to the information than anyone, do not seem to believe so. The questions get tougher from here.

 

It is possible than some of the contacts between the Ferguson PD and racial minorities there were lawful and fair? Is it possible that contact between Wilson and Brown was lawful and fair? And, is it possible that Brown’s actions precipitated and caused the end result? I have insufficient information to assess those questions. The DOJ findings certainly imply that the last answer is “Yes”. Nevertheless, how you may have just answered those in your head your have demonstrated one or more of the big pictures I explained above. The same applies if you believe there was absolutely no way Wilson or any other officer anywhere else could have done anything wrong.

 

In my first post on this blog, I cautioned against buying into absolutes because they can be misleading. I didn’t say all absolutes are wrong because that would itself be an absolute. If or when you find yourself thinking there is absolutely no way that this happened or that didn’t happen the way you believe, especially when all evidence is not in complete agreement, that can be a sign of trouble. It is also a fallacy known as generalizations. Such instances of flawed thought can be largely mitigated, though, by asking uncomfortable questions, by honestly trying to prove your beliefs wrong. This is an approach largely known as the scientific method. On the Brown killing, crime statistics, and a host of other issues big and small, I suspect very few people use that approach, in part because it is uncomfortable, inconvenient, and potentially-unpopular to name just three.

 

Former U.S. President James A. Garfield was quoted thusly: “The truth will set you free, but first it will make you miserable.” We all need to be prepared to be proven wrong no matter how miserable that may initially make us. That’s what the best scientists and investigators and journalists do, even when the results contradict what they first suspected the truth of their matter was. Without a willingness to accept that what we believe right now may be incorrect, a lot of nice people will be in a lot of trouble, in Ferguson and many other places, on their issue and a host of others. That is the biggest of the six big pictures here.

On Butler

In my last post, I mentioned English novelist Samuel Butler (1835 – 1902), who reportedly wrote, “A little knowledge is a dangerous thing,” That being said, I could be wrong.

 

I confess to being unable thus far to find in which of his works Butler write those words. Without such confirmation, I have to rely on a variety of web sources that may be incorrect. How can I know if some quotation sites copied that quote mistakenly from another site? Butler was also a satirist and in 1851 noted that, “The most important service rendered by the press and magazines is that of educating people to approach printed matter with distrust”. You needn’t speculate long on what he would have thought of unsourced web sites.

 

Continue reading On Butler

The truth is the truth, relatively speaking

 

In my recent book, The Full Scoop on BS, I praised the value of truth. The trouble with truth is that, like so many other aspects in life, it is often relative. This is not to devalue the truth. If anything, it is to be more truthful.

 

Those who write lengthy works such as theses, dissertations, or books know how easy it is to make spelling, typing and grammatical errors. Even repeated rewrites, editing, proof reading, and more editing can miss at least a few mistakes. I’ve found several in my recently published book, the worst so far being “heresay” instead of “hearsay”. It probably sounded right in my head but it should have looked wrong to my eyes. Although horrified to find errors, I was not surprised. Colleague and author Kerry Watkins warned me that no matter how many times someone else or I ran through my manuscript, errors would remain so at some point I would have to let my work out there, warts and all.

Continue reading The truth is the truth, relatively speaking